Skip navigation
Skip navigation

How bees distinguish colors

Horridge, George Adrian

Description

Behind each facet of the compound eye, bees have photoreceptors for ultraviolet, green, and blue wavelengths that are excited by sunlight reflected from the surrounding panorama. In experiments that excluded ultraviolet, bees learned to distinguish between black, gray, white, and various colors. To distinguish two targets of differing color, bees detected, learned, and later recognized the strongest preferred inputs, irrespective of which target displayed them. First preference was the position...[Show more]

dc.contributor.authorHorridge, George Adrian
dc.date.accessioned2015-12-08T22:22:51Z
dc.identifier.issn1179-2744
dc.identifier.urihttp://hdl.handle.net/1885/32639
dc.description.abstractBehind each facet of the compound eye, bees have photoreceptors for ultraviolet, green, and blue wavelengths that are excited by sunlight reflected from the surrounding panorama. In experiments that excluded ultraviolet, bees learned to distinguish between black, gray, white, and various colors. To distinguish two targets of differing color, bees detected, learned, and later recognized the strongest preferred inputs, irrespective of which target displayed them. First preference was the position and measure of blue reflected from white or colored areas. They also learned the positions and a measure of the green receptor modulation at vertical edges that displayed the strongest green contrast. Modulation is the receptor response to contrast and was summed over the length of a contrasting vertical edge. This also gave them a measure of angular width between outer vertical edges. Third preference was position and a measure of blue modulation. When they returned for more reward, bees recognized the familiar coincidence of these inputs at that place. They cared nothing for colors, layout of patterns, or direction of contrast, even at black/white edges. The mechanism is a new kind of color vision in which a large-field tonic blue input must coincide in time with small-field phasic modulations caused by scanning vertical edges displaying green or blue contrast. This is the kind of system to expect in medium-lowly vision, as found in insects; the next steps are fresh looks at old observations and quantitative models.
dc.publisherDovePRess
dc.rights© 2015 Horridge. This work is published by Dove Medical Press Limited, and licensed under Creative Commons Attribution – Non Commercial (unported, v3.0) License. The full terms of the License are available at http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc/3.0/. Non-commercial uses of the work are permitted without any further permission from Dove Medical Press Limited, provided the work is properly attributed. Permissions beyond the scope of the License are administered by Dove Medical Press Limited. Information on how to request permission may be found at: http://www.dovepress.com/permissions.php
dc.sourceEye and Brain
dc.titleHow bees distinguish colors
dc.typeJournal article
local.description.notesImported from ARIES
dc.date.issued2015
local.identifier.absfor060599 - Microbiology not elsewhere classified
local.identifier.ariespublicationu4008405xPUB94
local.type.statusPublished Version
local.contributor.affiliationHorridge, George Adrian, College of Medicine, Biology and Environment, ANU
local.bibliographicCitation.startpage17
local.bibliographicCitation.lastpage34
local.identifier.doi10.2147/EB.S77973
local.identifier.absseo970106 - Expanding Knowledge in the Biological Sciences
dc.date.updated2015-12-08T08:48:06Z
local.identifier.scopusID2-s2.0-84928982613
dcterms.accessRightsOpen Access
CollectionsANU Research Publications

Download

File Description SizeFormat Image
01_Horridge_How_bees_distinguish_col_2015.pdf1.06 MBAdobe PDF


Items in Open Research are protected by copyright, with all rights reserved, unless otherwise indicated.

Updated:  17 November 2022/ Responsible Officer:  University Librarian/ Page Contact:  Library Systems & Web Coordinator